Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Different associations between intelligence and social cognition in children with and without autism spectrum disorders

  • Tetsu Hirosawa ,

    Roles Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    hirosawatetsu1982@yahoo.co.jp

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan, Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Keiko Kontani,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan, Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Mina Fukai,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Masafumi Kameya,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Daiki Soma,

    Roles Data curation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Shoryoku Hino,

    Roles Formal analysis

    Affiliation Department of Neuropsychiatry, Ishikawa Prefectural Takamatsu Hospital, Ishikawa, Japan

  • Tatsuru Kitamura,

    Roles Formal analysis

    Affiliation Department of Neuropsychiatry, Ishikawa Prefectural Takamatsu Hospital, Ishikawa, Japan

  • Chiaki Hasegawa,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis

    Affiliation Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Kyung-min An,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Tetsuya Takahashi,

    Roles Data curation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Yuko Yoshimura,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan, Faculty of Education, Institute of Human and Social Sciences, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

  • Mitsuru Kikuchi

    Roles Data curation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan, Research Center for Child Mental Development, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan

Abstract

Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are characterized by impaired social cognition and communication. In addition to social impairment, individuals with ASD often have intellectual disability. Intelligence is known to influence the phenotypic presentation of ASD. Nevertheless, the relation between intelligence and social reciprocity in people with ASD remains unclear, especially in childhood. To elucidate this relation, we analyzed 56 typically developing children (35 male, 21 female, aged 60–91 months) and 46 children with ASD (35 male, 11 female, aged 60–98 months) from university and affiliated hospitals. Their cognitive function was evaluated using the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. Their social cognition was assessed using the Social Responsiveness Scale. We used linear regression models to ascertain whether the associations between intelligence and social cognition of typically developing children and children with ASD are significantly different. Among the children with ASD, scores on the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children correlated significantly with social cognition, indicating that higher intelligence is associated with better social cognition. For typically developing children, however, no significant correlation was found. One explanation might be that children with ASD fully use general intelligence for successful learning in social cognition, although extensive use of intelligence might not be necessary for TD children. Alternatively, autistic impairment in social cognition can be compensated by intelligence despite a persistent deficit in social cognition. In either case, when using the SRS as a quantitative phenotype measure for ASD, the influence of intelligence must be considered.

Introduction

Numerous and diverse difficulties driven by social impairment are experienced by individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a neurodevelopmental disorder associated with repetitive behaviors and characterized by impaired social cognition and communication [1]. For example, social impairment leads people with ASD to experience difficulties in education, employment, and in severe cases, independent living [25]. Moreover, it is noteworthy that severe social impairment in childhood strongly predicts those factors later in their adulthood [6], which emphasizes the importance of understanding factors associated with childhood social impairment.

Many individuals with ASD have intellectual disability (ID) in addition to social impairment [7]. Although shared etiologies are largely unknown, some researchers have inferred shared genetic risk factors [8] and prenatal risk factors such as exposure to infection [9], medications [10], and preterm delivery [11]. Furthermore, earlier reports describe that intelligence and autistic symptomatology might not be mutually independent. For instance, repetitive and restrictive behaviors (RRBs) have been shown to correlate with a lower nonverbal developmental quotient [1216], with the sole exception of a study by Joseph et al. which found no significant relation [17]. As another illustrative instance, Bishop et al. explored the relation between nonverbal IQ and RRBs in 830 children with ASD and reported that the prevalence of most RRBs (e.g. repetitive use of objects, and hand and finger mannerisms) is negatively associated with nonverbal IQ. Conversely, the association was positive for RBBs of certain types (e.g., circumscribed interests) [16].

In addition, the relation between social cognition and intelligence in ASD or PDDNOS has been debated since the 1990s [1820]. For instance, Buitelaar et al. examined 60 children: 20 children with autism; 20 with pervasive developmental disorder, not otherwise specified (PDDNOS); and 20 children under psychiatric control. Performance IQ and verbal memory were found to be strong predictors of performance on emotion recognition tasks. A significant positive correlation between performance IQ and second-order ToM was also reported, although the effect was not found to be significant for first-order ToM. They reported relationships throughout the sample, but also reported that the coefficient patterns did not differ markedly across the three diagnostic groups [18]. Subsequent studies of ASD and PDDNOS populations using various measures of social cognition almost invariably found significant association between better social cognition and higher intelligence [2129]. For instance, Livingston et al. measured ASD symptoms using ADOS-G and compared children with ASD having milder ASD symptoms despite poor ToM performance, with children who had severer ASD symptoms and poor ToM performance. They reported the former group as having higher intelligence [29]. One exception is a study conducted by Muller et al. They evaluated participants’ social cognition using a Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC), in which the participants inferred film characters’ mental states including emotions, thoughts, and intentions. Based on the MASC results, Muller et al. reported non-significant association between intelligence and social cognition [30].

Some studies have explored the relation between social cognition and intelligence in TD populations [22, 25, 28, 3033]. Unlike those in ASD populations, some reports have described results showing non-significant correlation [22, 3032]. Others have reported significant correlation [25, 28, 33]. Mohn et al. examined 250 healthy adults and reported non-significant association between IQ and social cognition, as measured using the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT) [31]. Farrelly et al. reported significant association between social cognition measured using the same battery (MSCEIT) and crystallized intelligence, as measured using the Gf/Gc Quickie Test Battery [33].

More generally, some other studies examined how autistic social impairment and intelligence are related. A few studies have examined the association between intelligence and social communication or motivation. Those studies examined ASD populations and found significant correlation between intelligence and social communication [3436]. Qualls et al. examined adolescents with ASD and reported positive association between scores of the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; [37]) and IQ [34]. To assess a general population, one study conducted with more than 5000 participants [38] revealed that higher verbal IQ has a significant association with better social communication, as measured by Social Communication Disorders Checklist and Children’s Communication Checklist, only in female participants. For social motivation, two studies have been conducted. Dawson et al. reported that lower IQ is related to lower social motivation, as measured by Broader Phenotype Autism Symptom Scale, in an ASD population [39]. Dubey et al. assessed social motivation by their “choose-a-movie paradigm measures” and reported that correlation was not significant for a combined population including both TD and ASD [40].

The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) [41] is a widely used rating scale for autistic symptomatology. It measures the severity of autistic symptomatology as a quantitative trait among children with ASD and among TD children. Actually, SRS comprises five sub-scales of the autistic mannerism sub-scale and four components of social reciprocity: receptive (social awareness sub-scale), cognitive (social cognition sub-scale), expressive (social communication sub-scale), and motivational (social motivation sub-scale) subdomains [41]. Those autistic traits and symptoms measured using SRS are distributed continuously, in ASD populations and in the general population [42]. Therefore, SRS is particularly useful for characterizing milder social impairment such as that which lies at the boundary between the normal population distribution and clinical-level affectation. In this sense, SRS might be suitable for examining the association between potentially milder autistic symptoms and intelligence. According to this framework, four studies have been conducted [4346]. Hus et al. recruited 2,368 individuals with ASD and 1,913 unaffected siblings. Subsequently, they assessed factors influencing the SRS raw scores. They reported the respective associations of higher SRS scores with greater non-ASD behavior difficulties, higher age, more impaired language, and lower non-verbal IQ in an ASD population. It is particularly interesting that non-verbal IQ effects on the SRS raw score were not found to be significant for unaffected siblings [45]. Three other recent studies specifically examined the sub-scales of SRS. The results are inconsistent. Rodgers et al. examined the association in a sample of children with ASD using the SRS-Second Edition (SRS-2; [47]) and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-4th Edition (WISC-IV; [48]). They reported that lower IQ is significantly associated with higher scores in the social cognition sub-scale (i.e., greater difficulties with social cognition) for girls only. No significant correlation was found for any other sub-scale [43]. Torske et al. assessed children with ASD or PDDNOS using the SRS [41]), age-appropriate Wechsler tests of intelligence, and Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF). They reported that IQ did not correlate to any sub-scale [44]. Chouinard et al. examined adolescent participants with and without ASD. They reported that higher IQ was correlated with a better social communication sub-scale of SRS [46].

Overall, studies of ASD subjects have tended to find significant association between intelligence and subdomains of social reciprocity. Studies of TD subjects have tended to find non-significant association. The relation in ASD children might differ from those in TD children. Actually, only one report describes a study examining this subject [22]. For that study, Kim et al. examined adolescent participants with and without ASD using a facial emotion recognition task in which participants identify six emotions expressed by avatars: happiness, fear, anger, disgust, sadness, and surprise. They reported that full scale IQ was positively correlated with accuracy of happy expression identification only in the ASD sample. Moreover, the group difference of the correlations was significant.

The study described herein was conducted to extend findings reported by Kim et al. [22] and those we described above to assess differences in the relations between intelligence and subdomains of social reciprocity in children with ASD and those in TD children. We narrowed the age range of the participants because social ability in childhood might influence the development of intelligence [49]. Consequently, the relation found for age-inhomogeneous participants might be unreliable. We hypothesize that the relations between intelligence and social reciprocity differ in children with ASD and in TD children. More specifically, higher intelligence is associated with better social awareness, social cognition, social communication, and social motivation only in children with ASD.

Materials and methods

Participants

From Kanazawa University and affiliated hospitals, we recruited 56 TD children (35 male, 21 female, aged 60–91 months) and 50 children with ASD (38 male, 12 female, aged 60–98 months). Four children with ASD were unable to complete the psychometric evaluation because of their severe psychomotor agitation. For those children, the experimenter decided to halt the evaluation. Their data were excluded from statistical analyses. Consequently, we analyzed 56 TD children and 46 children with ASD (35 male, 11 female, aged 60–98 months).

The ASD diagnosis was made according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth edition (DSM-IV) [1] using the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders (DISCO) [50] or the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic (ADOS-G) [51]. The exclusion criteria were (1) blindness, (2) deafness, (3) any other neuropsychiatric disorder including epilepsy, and (4) ongoing medication. Written informed consent was obtained from parents before participation by the children. The Ethics Committee of Kanazawa University Hospital approved the methods and procedures, all of which were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

We are continually recruiting participants as part of a single large project (Bambi plan, http://bambiplan.w3.kanazawa-u.ac.jp/pdf/jusen_english.pdf). Some participants overlap with those of our earlier study [52]. However, the results do not. In addition, the emphases of that earlier study differed from those of the present study.

Assessment of intelligence

For this study, we assessed the intelligence of the participants using the Japanese version of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) [53]. In K-ABC, the set of skills for problem-solving abilities is interpreted as intelligence. Knowledge of facts is defined as achievement. In this sense, K-ABC was developed to distinguish intelligence from knowledge [53, 54]. In K-ABC, intelligence is measured on the Sequential Processing Scale and the Simultaneous Processing Scale. The Mental Processing Scale (MPS), a unification of those two scales, is intended as a measure of total intelligence in the assessment battery [54]. On the Sequential Processing Scale, the child must solve problems in a serial, stepwise manner. The subtests included in this scale are Hand Movements, Number Recall, and Word Order. For the Simultaneous Processing Scale, the child must integrate many stimuli simultaneously to solve problems. The subtests included in this scale are Magic Window, Face Recognition, Gestalt Closure, Triangles, Matrix Analogies, Spatial Memory, and Photo Series. The Achievement Scale (ACH) provides an estimate of earlier learning. It reflects the effectiveness by which a child applies intelligence in real-life situations [55]. The subtests for ACH are Expressive Vocabulary, Faces and Places, Arithmetic, Riddles, Reading/Decoding, and Reading/Understanding. Kamphaus et al. reported that aCH includes measures of what have traditionally been identified as verbal intelligence (i.e., verbal concept formation and vocabulary) and acquired general information or school skills (arithmetic, letter and word reading, and word and sentence comprehension). We described details of the K-ABC sub-scales in S1 File. Some subtests are given at all ages. Others are given to selected age groups. Scores are provided as age-adjusted standardized scores, normalized to have mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15.

Haddad and Naglieri asserted that K-ABC differs from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Revised (WISC-R; [56]): WISC splits intelligence by verbal vs. non-verbal, but K-ABC measures intelligence based on the processing style necessary to solve tasks; moreover, it separates that assessment from achievement. Reported correlations of MPS with Full Scale IQ measured using the WISC-R tend to be moderate (0.62–0.76) [53]. Those results imply that intelligence measured using the K-ABC resembles that measured using the WISC-R, but they are not identical. In fact, they have their own unique characteristics. The unique characteristics of K-ABC might be a result derived from numerous factors including the following: (i) Limited verbalization is required of the child. In fact, K-ABC uses a minimal amount of verbal involvement in measuring intellectual processing [54]. (ii) The use of “teaching” items helps to ensure that all children understand what is expected of them for each task. (iii) The inclusion of a wider variety of tasks in the K-ABC than the variety found in WISC (e.g., Gestalt Closure, Face Recognition, Word Order). K-ABC is expected to be a good test battery for Children with ASD because they might have good intelligence irrespective of social or language impairment.

Assessment of social reciprocity

We assessed participants’ social reciprocity using SRS [41]: a 65-item rating scale that measures sociality and autistic mannerisms as a quantitative trait for TD children and for children who are clinically affected by autism spectrum conditions. It measures the social awareness sub-scale, social cognition sub-scale, social communication sub-scale, social motivation sub-scale, and autistic mannerisms sub-scale to generate a single measure. In both groups, a parent of each participant filled out the SRS. We used gender-normed T scores (SRS-T) of each sub-scale [57]. Higher scores represent greater difficulties with social cognition.

Although the validity of self-ratings of children is still under study, the SRS can be completed by a parent, a teacher, or another adult informant. In this way, it involves ratings of children in their natural social contexts and reflects what has been observed consistently over weeks or months of time rather than merely reflecting results of a single clinical or laboratory observation [41]. Therefore, it capitalizes on both direct observation and on the accumulated history of behaviors observed by the informant over time. By virtue of this characteristic, SRS reflects social abilities appearing not in only one-to-one real-world communication but also in one-to-many or many-to-many real-world communication, rather than those appearing in one-to-one communications or virtual communications in an examination room. For that reason, good agreement has been reported for comparison with other parents or teacher reported ASD-directed behavior assessments (e.g., SCQ [37, 58, 59]), Children’s Communication Checklist ([5860]), and Social and Communication Disorders Checklist [61]). The SRS scores are also known to exhibit high inter-rater reliability [62] and are known to be distributed continuously in a general population [42].

Statistical analysis

We tested differences in age and scores in K-ABC and SRS between TD and ASD using Student t-tests. Sex difference was tested using chi-square tests.

Statistical analysis–Regression analysis.

Before we applied linear regression, we verified that our data meet the assumptions for regression analysis. Specifically, we used standard methods to verify linearity, normality, homogeneity of variance, model specifications, influence, and collinearity. In some cases, the assumption of homogeneity was violated. Therefore, we used heteroscedasticity-robust standard errors [63].

First, to elucidate different relations between intelligence and social reciprocity in children with ASD from those in TD children, we applied linear regression models to predict the four SRS sub-scales (i.e., social awareness sub-scale, social cognition sub-scale, social communication sub-scale, social motivation sub-scale), respectively, based on the participants’ condition (i.e., ASD or TD), K-ABC sub-scales (i.e., MPS or ACH), age, and sex as well as their interaction. We incorporated age and sex in the models because of their possible influences on SRS scores [57, 64]. We did not examine relations between cognitive performance and the diagnostic group for the autistic mannerism sub-scale because our emphasis was to ascertain different relations between social reciprocity and intelligence.

Second, if a significant interaction effect was found, then we applied post-hoc analysis to elucidate the relation between SRS sub-scales and MPS or ACH further. We predicted the sub-scale based on MPS or ACH controlling for age and sex, respectively, in each condition (i.e., ASD or TD) using linear regression models.

Statistical analysis–Coarsened exact matching.

The results of simple regression analysis alone can be misleading if patient characteristics are very different between the conditions. In such cases, two options are available to address the imbalances: multiple regression and matching. For the detection of class effects, a regression model is often a more powerful tool than matching [65]. However, matching might be preferred in some cases (e.g., when the linearity assumption fails). To investigate the relations between social cognition and general intelligence or achievement further, we matched the groups in terms of MPS and ACH scores, age, and sex.

We improved the balance by coarsened exact matching (CEM). Then we performed adjusted regression analysis. In the CEM algorithm, we temporarily coarsen (or categorize) each variable based on its distribution or on natural or intuitive divisions. Each participant is then assigned to one of a specified set of strata in which the participant characteristics are exactly matched on a set of coarsened, or categorized variables. A weighting variable (CEM-weight) is generated to equalize the number of treated and control cases in one stratum. It is used for subsequent regression analysis [66].

We matched ASD and TD groups on MPS and ACH, age, and sex. As a binning algorithm, we used Scott’s rule [66]. This report describes the degree of imbalance between the two datasets before and after matching by measuring the multivariate L1 distance. The L1 distance, which is a value between zero and one, represents how two groups are balanced in terms of matched variables. Smaller values signify better balance. Larger values signify imbalance.

In subsequent regression analysis, we predict social cognition sub-scale based on the condition, and MPS or ACH as well as their interaction with CEM-weight for weighting. If a significant interaction effect was found, then we predicted social cognition sub-scale based respectively on MPS or ACH, in each condition.

For all statistical analyses, we inferred significance for P < .05. All statistical analyses were conducted using software (Stata ver. 15.0; Stata Corp., College Station, TX, USA).

Results

We found significant differences in age [t(100) = -2.96, p< .01], Mental Processing Scale [t(100) = 3.85, p < .01], Achievement Scale [t(100) = 5.05, p < .01, and SRS total score [t(100) = -11.0, p< .01], social awareness sub-scale [t(100) = -7.64, p< .01], social cognition sub-scale [t(100) = -9.33, p< .01], social communication sub-scale [t(100) = -9.71, p< .01], social motivation sub-scale [t(100) = -5.77, p< .01] between TD and ASD groups. Table 1 presents results. Children with ASD are older. They also had lower K-ABC and higher SRS sub-scale scores.

Group difference in associations between K-ABC sub-scales and SRS sub-scales–regression analysis

Significant interaction between condition and K-ABC sub-scales was found only in the model for the social cognition sub-scale. For the social cognition sub-scale, in the model using MPS, the interaction effect was found to be significant [t(96) = -2.33, p = .02]. The main effect of the condition [t(96) = 3.57, p < .01] was also found to be significant. No other factor was found to be significant. Similarly, in the model using ACH, the interaction effect was significant [t(96) = -2.19, p = .04]. A main effect of the condition [t(96) = 3.54, p < .01] was also significant. The results are presented in Table 2. The results in the models for other SRS sub-scales are presented in S1 Table in S1 File.

thumbnail
Table 2. Association between social cognition sub-scale and K-ABC sub-scales controlling for age and sex.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235380.t002

For reference purposes, we provide scatter plots and draw regression lines to visualize the associations between social cognition sub-scale and each K-ABC scale. For them, simple linear regressions were calculated to predict social cognition sub-scale based on MPS or ACH in each group (Figs 1 and 2). In the model using MPS, the main effect of the condition [t(98) = 3.43, p < .01] was found to be significant. The interaction effect was also significant [t(98) = -2.09, p = .04]. No other factor was found to be significant. In the model using ACH, the main effect of the condition [t(98) = 3.36, p < .01] was found to be significant. The interaction effect was also significant [t(98) = -2.03, p = .04]. No other factor was found to be significant. The results are presented in S2 Table in S1 File.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Relation between social cognition sub-scale scores and achievement scale scores on the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children.

TD, typically developing children; ASD, children with autism spectrum disorder.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235380.g001

thumbnail
Fig 2. Relation between social cognition sub-scale scores and mental processing scale scores on the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children.

TD, typically developing children; ASD, children with autism spectrum disorder.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235380.g002

Association between social cognition sub-scale and K-ABC scores in respective groups–regression analysis

To elucidate the relation between the social cognition sub-scale and general intelligence, or achievement, we applied linear regression to predict the social cognition sub-scale based on MPS or ACH, age, and sex in each group.

For TD children, in the model using MPS, a significant main effect of age was found [t(50) = -2.19, p = .03]. No other factor was found to be significant. A significant main effect of age was also significant [t(50) = -2.20, p = .03] in the model using ACH.

For children with ASD, in the model using MPS, significant main effects were found for MPS [t(42) = −3.68, p < .01], and sex [t(42) = -2.74, p < .01], and age [t(42) = 3.11, p < .01]. No other factor was found to be significant. Similar significant effects were identified in the model using ACH. Those results indicate that higher intelligence and achievement, younger age, and being male are associated with better social cognition in children with ASD. The results are presented in Table 3.

thumbnail
Table 3. Association between social cognition sub-scale and K-ABC sub-scales for respective groups controlling for age and sex.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235380.t003

Association between the social cognition sub-scale and K-ABC scores in respective groups, restricted to a sample with MPS of 90 or higher–regression analysis

Potentially, low general intelligence hinders social cognitive scores, but once a certain threshold is reached, increased intelligence does not provide any additional performance. Therefore, as an exploratory analysis, we excluded 27 participants with MPS lower than 90: 5 TD children and 22 children with ASD. Then we applied linear regression.

In a sample of participants with MPS of 90 or higher, differences in age, ACH, SRS total scores, and all the SRS sub-scales were found to be significant. S3 Table in S1 File presents the characteristics of the restricted sample. Children with ASD are older. They had lower ACH and higher SRS-T scores.

For this sample, we used linear regression to predict a social cognition sub-scale based on MPS or ACH, age, and sex, respectively, in each group. After controlling for age and sex effects, the associations between social cognition sub-scale and MPS or ACH were found to be significant in children with ASD. The results are presented in S4 Table in S1 File.

Association between K-ABC sub-scales and social cognition in matched participants–CEM

After improving balance using the CEM algorithm, 14 TD children and 9 children with ASD comprised the matched participants. The L1 distance improved from 0.828 to 0.288. Considering CEM weights, logistic regression showed sex differences as not significant [z(1) = -0.54, p = .59]. Similarly, regression models with CEM weights showed that the differences in age in months [t(23) = 0.56, p = .58], MPS score [t(23) = -0.31, p = .76], and ACH score [t(23) = 0.18, p = .86] were not significant in the matched sample. S5 Table in S1 File presents characteristics of the matched participants. S1 Fig presents a scatter plot and adjusted prediction using a 95% confidence interval.

After matching, we used linear regression with CEM weights to predict the social cognition sub-scale based on K-ABC scores (i.e., MPS and ACH) and condition (i.e., ASD or TD), as well as their interaction. For the model using MPS, the main effect of the condition [t(21) = 3.19, p < .01] was found to be significant. The interaction effect was also significant [t(21) = -2.68, p = .01]. In the model using ACH, the main effect of the condition [t(21) = 4.91, p < .001] and the interaction effect were found to be significant [t(21) = -4.03, p < .001]. The results are presented in Table 4.

thumbnail
Table 4. Association between the social cognition sub-scale and K-ABC scores in matched participants.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235380.t004

To elucidate the relation between social cognition and general intelligence, or achievement, we applied simple regression with CEM weights to predict the social cognition sub-scale based on MPS and ACH in each group. For TD children, the associations were not found to be significant. For children with ASD, the association between social cognition sub-scale and MPS was found to be significant [t(7) = -5.05, p < .01]. Similarly, the association between social cognition sub-scale and ACH was found to be significant [t(7) = -4.68, p < .01]. As in the case with unmatched participants, higher intelligence was associated with better social cognition.

Discussion

We found significant association between intelligence and social cognition in children with ASD. The association was not significant for TD children. This result is generally consistent with those of earlier studies exploring the relation between intelligence and social cognition in ASD [2128] and TD population [22, 25, 28, 3032]. Therefore, it conceptually replicates and extends earlier findings. One explanation for these associations might be that children with ASD fully capitalize on general intelligence for successful learning in social cognition, although extensive use of intelligence might not be necessary for TD children. Alternatively, autistic impairment in social cognition can be compensated by intelligence despite a continued deficit in basic abilities (e.g., ToM). In other words, children with ASD having higher intelligence can disguise underlying cognitive difficulties. In such cases, the social cognition sub-scale score can be better because SRS is reported by observers. This conjecture is in line with results presented in a recent report of work by Livingston et al. [29]. They compared children with ASD having milder symptoms, as measured by ADOS-G, despite poor ToM performance (i.e., those who disguise the underlying social cognitive difficulties) with those having severer symptoms and poor ToM performance (i.e., those who failed to disguise) and reported that the former group had higher intelligence. Considering the present results, the former group might have been able to disguise impaired ToM using higher intelligence, resulting in better scores in ADOS-G. Another explanation for the present result is that, as Hus and colleagues explained in a report of their study [45], SRS and its sub-scales measure more general cognitive ability than ASD traits specifically. From this perspective, it is noteworthy that the interactive term for the social communication sub-scale is very similar in magnitude to the social cognition sub-scale. The social communication sub-scale might also have been influenced by intelligence, even though it was not significant (S1b Table in S1 File: Interaction between diagnosis and ACH is beta = -.84; p = .052 for the social communication sub-scale vs. beta = -.099, p = .03 for the social cognition sub-scale). This conjecture is in line with results reported recently by Hus et al. [45]. Based on their large-scale study, they reported a significant relation between higher SRS-Raw total scores and lower non-verbal IQ, in addition to non-ASD-specific behavioral problems and cognitive skills in an ASD population. The present study might extend the results reported by Hus et al. in that the reported relation between SRS-Raw total scores and non-verbal IQ might have been driven by the influence of intelligence on social cognition and social communication sub-scales of SRS.

After restriction to a sample of individuals with MPS of 90 or higher, the association between lower intelligence and higher social cognition sub-scale (i.e., greater difficulties in social cognition) remained significant for children with ASD. Results of our study show a linear association between intelligence and social cognition. This result differs from those found in a study by Rommelse et al., which identified an inverted U-shaped relation (ASD with average IQ outperform in social cognition those with higher and lower IQ, as measured using the Amsterdam Neuropsychological Tasks (ANT) program) [67]. The difference might derive from the age-heterogeneity of participants. We recruited participants in a narrow age range (5–8 years old), although Rommelse et al. did not (6–21 years old). Considering the influence of childhood sociality on cognitive development over time [49], results from age-inhomogeneous populations might not reflect a true relation in childhood.

In our sample, female sex was associated significantly with higher social cognition sub-scale scores, which implies that girls have more difficulties related to social cognition than boys have. Sex differences in autistic symptomatology might be important considering biological and behavioral differences between girls and boys, but few reports have described effects of sex on social cognition in individuals with ASD. In terms of SRS, two reports have described effects of age on the social cognition sub-scale. Torske et al. reported a strong tendency for girls to have higher scores on the social cognition sub-scale [44], but Rodgers et al. reported a minimal effect size (Cohen’s d = -0.073) of sex [43]. Reports of other earlier studies using scales other than SRS tend to describe that female subjects with ASD have better social cognition than their male counterparts do. In fact, Isaksson et al. [26] and Muller et al. [30] measured social cognition respectively using RMET and MASC. They reported that female subjects with ASD performed better on those tests than male subjects with ASD did. Other reports have described non-significant results in terms of ToM [24, 27]. One explanation is expected to be that the current findings were driven by the few female subjects in the sample. Alternatively, considering that SRS T-scores are gender-normed but that the other assessments (RMET and MASC) are not, the gender-normed property of SRS T-scores might explain this contradiction: originally, gender-normed T-scores of SRS are developed to correct gender differences observed in normative samples [41]. Recently, however, Hus et al. examined a large sample of 2,368 children with ASD. They reported that gender effects on raw scores in SRS were minimal and not significant [45]. Given that the effects of gender on SRS raw scores are minimal, whereas the gender-normed T-scores are “corrected” for gender differences in the original normative samples, the use of gender-normed scores created an artificial appearance of greater social impairment in girls. Combining non-significant results from other non-gender-normed measures for social cognition, gender-normed SRS T-scores might have exaggerated difficulties in the female children with ASD in our sample.

We also found significant association between age and the social cognition sub-scale score: For TD children, younger age was associated with a higher social cognition sub-scale score (i.e., greater difficulties with social cognition). For children with ASD, however, older age was associated with higher social cognition sub-scale scores. Social cognition tends to improve with age in TD children [6871], but the SRS is normed so that age effects are not typically significant. In fact, the present result is contrary to those of most large-scale SRS studies of the general population, demonstrating non-significant age-related differences in SRS total scores. For example, Constantino and Todd examined a US population and found no effect of age on SRS raw scores [42]. Similar findings have been reported for German [71], Chinese [72], and Iranian [73] community and clinical samples. In fact, only for a Japanese population did Kamio et al. report a negative effect of age on SRS total scores (younger age was associated with higher SRS total scores) with a modest effect size [74]. The current study of this Japanese population found a similar relation between the age and social cognition sub-scale of SRS. Considering the present result, effects of age on SRS total scores observed by Kamio et al. might be driven by the effects exerted on the social cognition sub-scale. Moreover, the effects of age on the social cognition sub-scale in TD children might be different from country to country (e.g., the relation might be significant only in Japan). Additional research efforts must be undertaken to ascertain whether this relation is universal or culture-dependent.

By contrast, for children with ASD, the present results demonstrated that older age is associated with higher scores on the social cognition sub-scale (i.e., greater difficulties with social cognition). This result is similar to those reported from the study by Hus et al. They reported significant association between higher SRS total scores (i.e., greater difficulties with social reciprocity) and older age in children with ASD [45]. Considering the results presented herein, this relation might be driven by effects of age on the social cognition sub-scale. However, other earlier studies have tended to show significant and positive effects of age on social cognition measured from a single clinical or laboratory observation. For example, older age was associated with better performance on laboratory test measures including ToM [27, 28], social analogical reasoning [69], RMET [26], and facial emotion recognition [21]. In addition to those one-to-one laboratory measures, the scores of Theory of Mind Inventory (ToMI), a parent report measure for theory of mind and social cognition, tend to improve with age [75]. Why is the social cognition sub-scale in SRS biased to attribute more social cognitive ability to younger children with ASD, even though their older counterparts are expected to have higher skills for one-to-one communication (e.g., better performance in ToM, social analogical reasoning, and RMET) and theory of mind? To explain that discrepancy, one must note that our sample comprises children with mean (standard deviation) age of 73.1(10.3) and that children in Japan attend elementary school from the April following their sixth birthday. In fact, in our sample, the older children are elementary school age children. After entering elementary school, education starts to be conducted while they are living in a group. Teachers encourage children to construct close and personal relationships with their classmates. Under these circumstances, latent difficulties of social cognition in children with ASD might begin to surface. Such difficulties might negatively affect parents’ perceptions of social cognition, yielding higher scores on the social cognition sub-scale, and exceeding some advancement in skills for one-to-one communications or theory of mind.

For the current study, we extended earlier findings by examining different associations between intelligence and social cognition in children with ASD versus TD children. Importantly, significant association was found only in children with ASD. For further progress, in light of the developmental aspects of ASD, a longitudinal follow-up study should be conducted to assess the relation between intelligence and social cognition. Change in social cognition over the course of time in relation to intelligence can be expected to provide additional information related to the structure of social cognition in this population. We found a significant effect of intelligence only on the social cognition sub-scale of SRS, although the effect on social communication sub-scale was similar in magnitude. These results indicate a complex relation between intelligence and social reciprocity in children with and without ASD. When using SRS as a measure of social reciprocity, the different influences of intelligence on the respective sub-scales must be considered.

Some limitations are noteworthy. First, we excluded children with known ID. Therefore, these study findings might not be applicable to a typical ASD population that includes comorbid ID. Second, although children having other psychiatric disorders were excluded from the study, the participants still possibly had comorbid disorders that affect cognitive performance because developmental disorders such as attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and learning disability are difficult to detect at this age. Third, the study participants were recruited from a small region. Considering environmental risk factors for ASD, our results should be generalized with caution. Fourth, we used parent report-based measures only. Considering the different results derived from laboratory and informant-based measures, future research should combine those two measures because they are mutually complementary [76].

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the efforts of our laboratory colleagues including Daisuke Saito, Takashi Ikeda, Ken Yaoi, Mutsumi Ozawa, Yoko Morita, and Sachiko Kitagawa. This work was supported by the Center of Innovation Program of the Japan Science and Technology Agency, JST.

References

  1. 1. American Psychiatric Association [APA]. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR). American Psychiatric Association. 2000. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890423349
  2. 2. Anderson DK, Liang JW, Lord C. Predicting young adult outcome among more and less cognitively able individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2014. pmid:24313878
  3. 3. Baron-Cohen S. The Autistic Child’s Theory of Mind: a Case of Specific Developmental Delay. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1989. pmid:2523408
  4. 4. Howlin P, Goode S, Hutton J, Rutter M. Adult outcome for children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004. pmid:14982237
  5. 5. Levy A, Perry A. Outcomes in adolescents and adults with autism: A review of the literature. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2011.
  6. 6. Howlin P, Moss P, Savage S, Rutter M. Social outcomes in mid- to later adulthood among individuals diagnosed with autism and average nonverbal IQ as children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013. pmid:23702446
  7. 7. Henninger NA, Taylor JL. Outcomes in adults with autism spectrum disorders: A historical perspective. Autism. 2013. pmid:22914775
  8. 8. Nishiyama T, Taniai H, Miyachi T, Ozaki K, Tomita M, Sumi S. Genetic correlation between autistic traits and IQ in a population-based sample of twins with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). J Hum Genet. 2009. pmid:19158814
  9. 9. McDermott S, Daguise V, Mann H, Szwejbka L, Callaghan W. Perinatal risk for mortality and mental retardation associated with maternal urinary-tract infections. J Fam Pract. 2001; 50:433–437. pmid:11350709
  10. 10. Christensen J, Grnøborg TK, Srøensen MJ, Schendel D, Parner ET, Pedersen LH et al. Prenatal valproate exposure and risk of autism spectrum disorders and childhood autism. JAMA–J Am Med Assoc. 2013. pmid:23613074
  11. 11. Abel KM, Dalman C, Svensson AC, Susser E, Dal H, Idring S et al. Deviance in fetal growth and risk of autism spectrum disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2013. pmid:23545793
  12. 12. Bishop SL, Hus V, Duncan A, Huerta M, Gotham K, Pickles A et al. Subcategories of restricted and repetitive behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013. pmid:23065116
  13. 13. Richler J, Huerta M, Bishop SL, Lord C. Developmental trajectories of restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests in children with autism spectrum disorders. Dev Psychopathol. 2010. pmid:20102647
  14. 14. Cuccaro ML, Shao Y, Grubber J, Slifer M, Wolpert CM, Donnelly SL et al. Factor analysis of restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism using the autism diagnostic interview-R. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2003.
  15. 15. Gabriels RL, Cuccaro ML, Hill DE, Ivers BJ, Goldson E. Repetitive behaviors in autism: Relationships with associated clinical features. Res Dev Disabil. 2005. pmid:15590247
  16. 16. Bishop SL, Richler J, Lord C. Association between restricted and repetitive behaviors and nonverbal IQ in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Child Neuropsychol. 2006. pmid:16911971
  17. 17. Joseph L, Thurm A, Farmer C, Shumway S. Repetitive behavior and restricted interests in young children with autism: Comparisons with controls and stability over two years. Autism Res. 2013. pmid:23868881
  18. 18. Buitelaar JK, Van Der Wees M, Swaab-Barneveld H, Van Der Gaag RJ. Verbal memory and performance IQ predict theory of mind and emotion recognition ability in children with autistic spectrum disorders and in psychiatric control children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 1999.
  19. 19. Happé FGE. Wechsler IQ Profile and Theory of Mind in Autism: A Research Note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994. pmid:7868640
  20. 20. Yirmiya N, Solomonica-Levi D, Shulman C, Pilowsky T. Theory of mind abilities in individuals with autism, Down syndrome, and mental retardation of unknown etiology: The role of age and intelligence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 1996. pmid:9119934
  21. 21. Garman HD, Spaulding CJ, Webb SJ, Mikami AY, Morris JP, Lerner MD. Wanting it Too Much: An Inverse Relation Between Social Motivation and Facial Emotion Recognition in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2016. pmid:26743637
  22. 22. Kim K, Rosenthal MZ, Gwaltney M, Jarrold W, Hatt N, McIntyre N et al. A Virtual Joy-Stick Study of Emotional Responses and Social Motivation in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015. pmid:24435405
  23. 23. Hollocks MJ, Jones CRG, Pickles A, Baird G, Happé F, Charman T et al. The Association Between Social Cognition and Executive Functioning and Symptoms of Anxiety and Depression in Adolescents With Autism Spectrum Disorders. Autism Res. 2014. pmid:24737743
  24. 24. Sasson NJ, Morrison KE, Kelsven S, Pinkham AE. Social cognition as a predictor of functional and social skills in autistic adults without intellectual disability. Autism Res. 2020. pmid:31419075
  25. 25. Morrison KE, Pinkham AE, Kelsven S, Ludwig K, Penn DL, Sasson NJ. Psychometric Evaluation of Social Cognitive Measures for Adults with Autism. Autism Res. 2019. pmid:30770676
  26. 26. Isaksson J, Taylor MJ, Lundin K, Neufeld J, Bölte S. Familial confounding on the ability to read minds: A co-twin control study. Autism. 2019. pmid:30895802
  27. 27. Kenny L, Hill E, Hamilton AF de C. The Relationship between Social and Motor Cognition in Primary School Age-Children. Front Psychol. 2016. pmid:26941685
  28. 28. Peterson CC, Slaughter V, Brownell C. Children with autism spectrum disorder are skilled at reading emotion body language. J Exp Child Psychol. 2015. pmid:26079273
  29. 29. Livingston LA, Colvert E, Bolton P, Happé F. Good social skills despite poor theory of mind: exploring compensation in autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2019. pmid:29582425
  30. 30. Müller N, Baumeister S, Dziobek I, Banaschewski T, Poustka L. Validation of the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition in Adolescents with ASD: Fixation Duration and Pupil Dilation as Predictors of Performance. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016. pmid:27271932
  31. 31. Mohn C, Sundet K, Rund BR. The relationship between IQ and performance on the MATRICS consensus cognitive battery. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2014. pmid:29379744
  32. 32. August SM, Kiwanuka JN, McMahon RP, Gold JM. The MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery (MCCB): Clinical and cognitive correlates. Schizophr Res. 2012. pmid:22093182
  33. 33. Farrelly D, Austin EJ. Ability EI as an intelligence? Associations of the MSCEIT with performance on emotion processing and social tasks and with cognitive ability. Cogn Emot. 2007.
  34. 34. Qualls LR, Corbett BA. Examining the relationship between social communication on the ADOS and real-world reciprocal social communication in children with ASD. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2017. pmid:28947912
  35. 35. Pecukonis M, Plesa Skwerer D, Eggleston B, Meyer S, Tager-Flusberg H. Concurrent Social Communication Predictors of Expressive Language in Minimally Verbal Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019. pmid:31187332
  36. 36. Bishop SL, Havdahl KA, Huerta M, Lord C. Subdimensions of social-communication impairment in autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2016. pmid:26748412
  37. 37. Rutter M, Bailey A, Lord C. The Social Communication Questionnaire: Manual. Western Psychological Services; 2003.
  38. 38. Skuse DH, Mandy W, Steer C, Miller LL, Goodman R, Lawrence K et al. Social communication competence and functional adaptation in a general population of children: Preliminary evidence for sex-by-verbal IQ differential risk. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009. pmid:19106766
  39. 39. Dawson G, Estes A, Munson J, Schellenberg G, Bernier R, Abbott R. Quantitative assessment of autism symptom-related traits in probands and parents: Broader phenotype autism symptom scale. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007. pmid:16868845
  40. 40. Dubey I, Ropar D, De C Hamilton AF. Measuring the value of social engagement in adults with and without autism. Mol Autism. 2015. pmid:26097674
  41. 41. Constantino J, Gruber C. Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2005.
  42. 42. Constantino JN, Todd RD. Autistic Traits in the General Population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003. pmid:12742874
  43. 43. Rodgers JD, Lodi-Smith J, Donnelly JP, Lopata C, McDonald CA, Thomeer ML et al. Brief Report: Examination of Sex-Based Differences in ASD Symptom Severity among High-Functioning Children with ASD Using the SRS-2. J Autism Dev Disord. 2019. pmid:30151783
  44. 44. Torske T, Nærland T, Øie MG, Stenberg N, Andreassen OA. Metacognitive aspects of executive function are highly associated with social functioning on parent-rated measures in children with autism spectrum disorder. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018. pmid:29375332
  45. 45. Hus V, Bishop S, Gotham K, Huerta M, Lord C. Factors influencing scores on the social responsiveness scale. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2013. pmid:22823182
  46. 46. Chouinard B, Gallagher L, Kelly C. He said, she said–Autism spectrum diagnosis and gender differentially affect relationships between executive functions and social communication. Autism. 2019. pmid:30823851
  47. 47. Constantino JN, Gruber C. Social Responsiveness Scale, Second Edition (SRS-2). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2012.
  48. 48. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (4th edition). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2003.
  49. 49. Vivanti G, Barbaro J, Hudry K, Dissanayake C, Prior M. Intellectual Development in Autism Spectrum Disorders: New Insights from Longitudinal Studies. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013. pmid:23847518
  50. 50. Wing L, Leekam SR, Libby SJ, Gould J, Larcombe M. The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders: Background, inter-rater reliability and clinical use. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2002. pmid:11944874
  51. 51. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, Dilavore PC et al. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000.
  52. 52. Hirosawa T, Kikuchi M, Fukai M, Hino S, Kitamura T, An K-M et al. Association Between Magnetoencephalographic Interictal Epileptiform Discharge and Cognitive Function in Young Children With Typical Development and With Autism Spectrum Disorders. Front Psychiatry. 2018;9: 1–9. pmid:29410632
  53. 53. Kaufman A, Kaufman N. Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1983.
  54. 54. Kaufman AS, O’neal MR, Avant AH, Long SW. Review Article: Introduction to the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) for Pediatric Neuroclinicians. Journal of Child Neurology. 1987. pmid:3624826
  55. 55. Kamphaus RW, Reynolds CR. Development and structure of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. J Spec Educ. 1984.
  56. 56. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. New York: The Psychological Corporation; 1974.
  57. 57. Constantino JN, Davis SA, Todd RD, Schindler MK, Gross MM, Brophy SL et al. Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. J Autism Dev Disord. 2003.
  58. 58. Charman T, Baird G, Simonoff E, Loucas T, Chandler S, Meldrum D et al. Efficacy of three screening instruments in the identification of autistic-spectrum disorders. Br J Psychiatry. 2007. pmid:18055961
  59. 59. Pine DS, Guyer AE, Goldwin M, Towbin KA, Leibenluft E. Autism spectrum disorder scale scores in pediatric mood and anxiety disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008. pmid:18434923
  60. 60. Bishop DVM. Development of the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 1998.
  61. 61. Bölte S, Westerwald E, Holtmann M, Freitag C, Poustka F. Autistic traits and autism spectrum disorders: The clinical validity of two measures presuming a continuum of social communication skills. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011. pmid:20422277
  62. 62. Constantino JN, Yang D, Gray TL, Gross MM, Abbacchi AM, Smith SC et al. Clarifying the associations between language and social development in autism: A study of non-native phoneme recognition. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007. pmid:17080273
  63. 63. White H. A Heteroskedasticity-Consistent Covariance Matrix Estimator and a Direct Test for Heteroskedasticity. Econometrica. 1980. pmid:12261749
  64. 64. Constantino JN, Abbacchi AM, Lavesser PD, Reed H, Givens L, Chiang L et al. Developmental course of autistic social impairment in males. Dev Psychopathol. 2009. pmid:19144226
  65. 65. Brazauskas R, Logan BR. Observational Studies: Matching or Regression? Biol Blood Marrow Transplant. 2016. pmid:26712591
  66. 66. Blackwell M, Iacus S, King G, Porro G. Cem: Coarsened exact matching in Stata. Stata J. 2009.
  67. 67. Rommelse N, Langerak I, Van Der Meer J, De Bruijn Y, Staal W, Oerlemans A et al. Intelligence may moderate the cognitive profile of patients with ASD. PLoS One. 2015. pmid:26444877
  68. 68. Chiasson V, Vera-Estay E, Lalonde G, Dooley JJ, Beauchamp MH. Assessing social cognition: age-related changes in moral reasoning in childhood and adolescence. Clin Neuropsychol. 2017. pmid:28080301
  69. 69. Green AE, Kenworthy L, Gallagher NM, Antezana L, Mosner MG, Krieg S et al. Social analogical reasoning in school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder and typically developing peers. Autism. 2017. pmid:27178998
  70. 70. Kessels RPC, Montagne B, Hendriks AW, Perrett DI, De Haan EHF. Assessment of perception of morphed facial expressions using the Emotion Recognition Task: Normative data from healthy participants aged 8–75. J Neuropsychol. 2014. pmid:23409767
  71. 71. Bölte S, Holtmann M, Poustka F. The social communication questionnaire (SCQ) as a screener for autism spectrum disorders: Additional evidence and cross-cultural validity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2008. pmid:18496329
  72. 72. Cen CQ, Liang YY, Chen QR, Chen KY, Deng HZ, Chen BY et al. Investigating the validation of the Chinese Mandarin version of the Social Responsiveness Scale in a Mainland China child population. BMC Psychiatry. 2017. pmid:28166747
  73. 73. Tehrani-Doost M, Shahrivar Z, Torabi N, Ansari S, Haji-Esmaeelzadeh M, Saeed-Ahmadi S. Cross-Cultural Validation and Normative Data of the Social Responsiveness Scale in a Group of Iranian General Child Population. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018. pmid:30334127
  74. 74. Kamio Y, Inada N, Moriwaki A, Kuroda M, Koyama T, Tsujii H et al. Quantitative autistic traits ascertained in a national survey of 22 529 Japanese schoolchildren. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2013. pmid:23171198
  75. 75. Hutchins TL, Prelock PA, Bonazinga L. Psychometric evaluation of the Theory of Mind Inventory (ToMI): A study of typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012. pmid:21484516
  76. 76. Leung RC, Vogan VM, Powell TL, Anagnostou E, Taylor MJ. The role of executive functions in social impairment in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Child Neuropsychol. 2016. pmid:25731979